Fourth, the 85th percentile argument so beloved of speed advocates is also disproved by American experience. This argument holds that speed limits should be set to the 85th
percentile of measured median speeds, regardless of existing speed limits. Speed advocates
contend that raising the limit would simply accomodate the natural feel of the road, and
bring most drivers into compliance with the new limit.
The reality is that 85th percentile speeds are usually 10 to 20 kmh faster than the
limit, and represent the driver's judgement of the fastest speed he can get away with,
rather than some natural characteristic of the road.
American experience has been that, when speed limits increase, so do the 85th percentile measured speeds. My own experience on roads in the north of Victoria where
the speed limit was lifted from 100 kph to 110 kph also concur with this observation.
Even worse, America found that the proportion of extremely fast speedsters increases disproportionately.
Are recidivist drivers the reason for the plateau?
Advertisement
There are alternative explanations for the plateauing of crash rates, but Buckingham does not consider them. The most likely is simply that education and advocacy campaigns
have reached all reasonable people, and are now stalled at a small hard core of drivers. Most population based trends follow similar patterns.
Another possible factor is that mobile phone usage, which started to become prominent in the mid 1990's, caused more crashes. Many studies have confirmed early findings by
the New England Journal of Medicine that talking while driving increases crash risk four times, although mobile phone companies in America have tried to deny and obfuscate
this link.
The rights of the rest of us
Crashes and run-downs generally occur when a driver fails to respond quickly enough
to some unexpected event, such as a vehicle suddenly entering from a side street or a
child running out from between parked cars. The faster the vehicle is travelling, the
less time the driver has to avoid impact. This is indisputable.
On high speed 2-lane roads, the speedster is deadly for he or she engages in lots of overtaking, which exposes innocent oncoming traffic to combined speeds typically over
230 kmh.
Studies by professional road safety researchers in Australia have found that crashes
and road deaths unequivocally decline where speed cameras are used. Evaluations by ARRB
Transport Research, for example, found that fatalities declined by
74 percent at ACT sites, and by 95 percent at a
sample of 28 sites in NSW. When Victoria
introduced cameras in 1989, crashes dropped 46 percent over the succeeding three years. Buckingham airily dismisses this outstanding result by claiming the preceding year was unusual.
Speed advocates believe they should be trusted to make the judgement as to what speed is safe and preferable. Unfortunately, what seems safe to a business executive
running late for a meeting is probably not safe to other parties, including lawful drivers, cyclists, walkers, children, parents, police and medical practitioners. That
is why we have speed limits.
Advertisement
Public acceptance of speed cameras is high, according to the ARRB Transport Research study, with only 5-7 per cent of people surveyed saying the cameras provide no benefits. In America, a
telephone survey of 6,000 drivers in 1997 found that 27 percent of drivers felt there was too little police enforcement on interstate roads and 40 percent felt that way about residential roads. Only 4 to 5 percent felt there was too much enforcement.
The pernicious elements of speeding ideology
Speed cameras attract widespread ire from speedsters for two reasons - cameras are effective and they're unbiased. They don't care whether the speedster is a politician,
or is well dressed, or is driving an expensive car.
The result has been a range of attacks and rhetorical devices intended to shift blame from speeding, such as claims that drivers have better concentration when speeding
and that lawful drivers are less skillful. The reality is that reduced response times don't translate to quicker or better responses. Further, it is speeding that requires less skill, not lawful driving. Any fool can put their foot down and pass other cars. It takes skill to maintain station in a fluid traffic environment, and skill to maintain
a reserve of time and speed to handle untoward events.
Buckingham tries to impute the great decline in crashes from 1980 to something he calls a driving culture, which by implication is independent from road safety and
enforcement programs. The fact is that the decline in crashes was shaped and driven by the very enforcement programs Buckingham is now complaining about. What's more,
each time a new program such as drink driving was introduced, libertarian forces complained about and tried to stifle them. In America, driver groups still complain about seat belts and air bags. Buckingham argues that improvements in car safety
created the safety improvements, yet America had the same car and road improvements, without the improvements in crash rates that we saw.
Some of Buckingham's arguments against speed cameras are blatant threats of unlawful behaviour, which sit oddly with the speed lobby's claims to wronged law-abiding
citizens. These include threats to race through suburban streets to avoid cameras, to speed elsewhere to "make up time," and to use false number plates to avoid detection.
Conclusion
Buckingham's interpretation of crash statistics does not support his contention which, in any case, seems to largely represent a standard polemic of speed lobbyists. The evidence from professional road safety researchers is clear that speed cameras save lives.