Like what you've read?

On Line Opinion is the only Australian site where you get all sides of the story. We don't
charge, but we need your support. Here�s how you can help.

  • Advertise

    We have a monthly audience of 70,000 and advertising packages from $200 a month.

  • Volunteer

    We always need commissioning editors and sub-editors.

  • Contribute

    Got something to say? Submit an essay.


 The National Forum   Donate   Your Account   On Line Opinion   Forum   Blogs   Polling   About   
On Line Opinion logo ON LINE OPINION - Australia's e-journal of social and political debate

Subscribe!
Subscribe





On Line Opinion is a not-for-profit publication and relies on the generosity of its sponsors, editors and contributors. If you would like to help, contact us.
___________

Syndicate
RSS/XML


RSS 2.0

Of mice and men: when peer review fails

By Don Aitkin - posted Monday, 18 February 2013


I've come across a fascinating story in the field of medical research. It is about the use of mice and rats as research subjects for diseases in humans, a role that they have played for decades. The story has two parts, the study itself, and the difficulty the authors had in getting it published. The second explains the title I have used, but the first is interesting in itself.

Mice have been the 'species of choice' for those who study human diseases, which explains why research centres that do this work have, and have to have, a controlled environment in which mice are bred, and maintained. These environments can be very expensive. We use mice, rats and rabbits, as experimental animals because they breed abundantly. Humans and mice share 99 per cent of their genes and 97.5 per cent of their DNA. And we have learned a great deal from the use of mice, to the point where they provide the standard vehicle for experimental research into human disease.

The study in question found that in three critical areas - sepsis, burns and trauma - the mouse model simply didn't work. Every one of nearly 150 drugs tested at great expense in humans with sepsis failed. The drugs were all based on studies on mice, and it seems to be the case that the sepsis that mice have is not the same as the human variety. One reason is that mice scavenge on food material that humans cold not tolerate, and therefore can handle bacterial infection more easily than humans. Mice need a million times more bateria in their blood than would kill a human being.

Advertisement

Was sepsis (a potentially deadly reaction from the body when infection sets in, and the leading cause of death to patients in intensive-care units) the same in mice as it was in humans? Over ten years the researchers collected data from humans and compared what they had with data from mice. They discovered that there were no similarities: humans and mice dealt with sepsis through different mechanisms - a gene that might be used in mice was suppressed in humans, and so on.

What is more, different conditions in mice - burns, trauma and sepsis - used different groups of genes, while in humans similar genes were used in all three conditions. For humans, then, you might find a drug that worked for all three conditions, but that wouldn't be the case for mice.

When they tried to discuss their findings, and then to publish them, there were objections that they had not shown that the same gene response also occurred in mice!

'They were so used to doing mouse studies that they thought that was how you validate things,' the lead author of the study said. 'They are so ingrained in trying to cure mice that they forget we are trying to cure humans.'

Their paper argued that there was no relationship between the genetic responses of mice and those of human beings, and journal after journal rejected it, including both Science and Nature. According to one of the authors, reviewers from the journals that rejected the paper did not point to scientific errors. In his words, 'the most common response was, "It has to be wrong. I don't know why it is wrong, but it has to be wrong.'"

Eventually they had the paper published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, an option available to one of the authors, who was a member. Now that the paper is out, there is a chorus of amazement from within the research community.

Advertisement

'When I read the paper, I was stunned by just how bad the mouse data are,' said one researcher. 'It's really amazing - no correlation at all. These data are so persuasive and so robust that I think funding agencies are going to take note. Until now, to get funding, you had to propose experiments using the mouse model.'

It is not the end of the line for mice as research subjects, but it is the end of a widespread assumption that mice and men are, at least in the research environment, virtually interchangeable. And that should mean that we get better outcomes for pre-clinical research in these areas.

And it should remind us again that the world of peer review is governed by widespread assumptions about how things actually work, assumptions that aren't always correct. I've made this point before about climate change, and about peer review itself. It is a necessary instrument in deciding whether or not a given research proposal should be funded, or a given paper should be published, but it is not at all infallible, and it operates on assumptions of all kinds.

As it happens, one of the authors of the study in question is a Dr Warren, who researches sepsis at Massachusetts General Hospital. Another Dr Warren, senior pathologist at the Royal Perth Hospital, was one of the two discoverers of helicobacter pylori, the cause of stomach ulcers, for which they were later to win the Nobel Prize.

They had a lot of trouble in getting their research accepted and published, too. It was assumed that it was stress that was the main cause of those ulcers, as I know to my cost. I had a dangerous operation, which altered a good deal of my pleasure in eating and drinking, in order to deal with this imagined stress. A few years later my condition would have been dealt with orally, cheaply and quickly.

I can give only two cheers for peer review, as I said in my first essay on it.

  1. Pages:
  2. Page 1
  3. All

This article was first published on Don Aitkin.



Discuss in our Forums

See what other readers are saying about this article!

Click here to read & post comments.

54 posts so far.

Share this:
reddit this reddit thisbookmark with del.icio.us Del.icio.usdigg thisseed newsvineSeed NewsvineStumbleUpon StumbleUponsubmit to propellerkwoff it

About the Author

Don Aitkin has been an academic and vice-chancellor. His latest book, Hugh Flavus, Knight was published in 2020.

Other articles by this Author

All articles by Don Aitkin

Creative Commons LicenseThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons License.

Article Tools
Comment 54 comments
Print Printable version
Subscribe Subscribe
Email Email a friend
Advertisement

About Us Search Discuss Feedback Legals Privacy